SAFETYLIT WEEKLY UPDATE

We compile citations and summaries of about 400 new articles every week.
RSS Feed

HELP: Tutorials | FAQ
CONTACT US: Contact info

Search Results

Journal Article

Citation

Dominick JR. Journal. Q. 1974; 51(1): 5-12.

Copyright

(Copyright © 1974, American Association of Schools and Departments of Journalism)

DOI

unavailable

PMID

unavailable

Abstract

VioLit summary:

OBJECTIVE:
The purpose of this study by Dominick was to examine relationships between media exposure to crime and law enforcement and attitudes about police and crime among elementary school children. It also attempted to bridge the gap between research that has focused solely on mass media socialization and research that has focused only on young people's attitudes about police and crime.

METHODOLOGY:
During May of 1972, questionnaires were completed by 371 fifth graders in New York City schools. Two schools were in blue collar communities and two were in white collar communities. A content analysis was done to pick "crime shows" aired in the 1970-1971 television season. Nothing was mentioned about how the schools, children, or TV sample episodes were chosen. Crime shows were defined as shows that had at least two crimes in the sample episode. Respondents were asked about how often they watched these shows. Scaling went from 1 (never) to 4 (almost always watch). The Crime Show Viewing Index was used as an independent variable, and is calculated by simply summing these weights for the 21 crime shows.
The TV Importance Index was also used as an independent variable. In a pretest, respondents were asked about their most important source of information about police, crime,and law enforcement. Four sources of information (TV, parents, friends, and personal contact with police) were used in a pair comparison technique adapted from White. The number of times TV was chosen as the most important source was then tabulated, with scores ranging from 3 (high importance) to 0 (low importance). Perceived attitudes of friends toward police was also used as an independent variable. This was indexed by three inter-correlated items. Weights were assigned to each option and summed. Parents' attitudes towards police was also used as an independent variable. This measure used two items with revised wording from the Friend's Index. Personal contact with police was used as an independent variable and an index was created from three yes-no questions concerning the respondent's experience with police. The Occupational Prestige scale was also used. This is a 13 step index described by Troldahl.
Eight questions were then asked designed to measure attitudes and knowledge about police. These were the dependent variables: identification with TV characters, occupational prestige ranking, evaluation of police, efficacy of police, reality of TV police, perceptions of crime and police violence, reporting of witnessed crimes, and knowledge of the legal system. For criterion variables measured by nominal methods, independent variables were dichotomized by means of a median split and chi square tests were performed. For ordinal techniques, variables were transformed into ordinal scales and Spearman rhos were calculated. Product moment correlations were used where interval scales were the criterion variables.

FINDINGS/DISCUSSION:
The inter-correlation among the six dependent variables was generally low. Contact with police was significantly correlated with crime show viewing (CSV) (p<.001). The contact variable was also related to perceived attitudes of friends, which, in turn was correlated with parental attitudes. Identification: More frequent viewers of police shows (both girls and boys) were significantly more likely to name a TV character associated with law enforcement as their identification choice than were less frequent viewers. Prestige: Among both boys and girls, no dependent variable was correlated with the prestige rankings of either policeman or private detective. Reality of TV police: The strongest predictor was perceived attitudes of friends (significant at .01). Among boys only, there was, as expected, a significant negative correlation between actual contact with police and perceived reality. Evaluation of police: The strongest predictors of this measure were attitudes of parents and friends. The youngsters attitudes, whether positive or negative, were consistent with those attitudes they attributed to friends and family. In the boys sample, more personal contact with police was related to more negative attitudes toward police. Efficacy: High crime show viewers expressed significantly greater agreement that criminals usually get caught. (68% of the high CSV group compared to 52% of the low CSV group). .05 is the assumed level of significance. Perceived parental attitudes were also positively and significantly related to the variable in the boys sample. Among those boys who view their parents' attitudes toward police as negative (p<.05). Among those of the positive group (p<.05). Frequency of crime and police violence: Contrary to predictions, no dependent variable was related to responses to these items in either sample. Reporting of witnessed crimes: There was a significant relationship between the television importance index and the reporting of witnessed crimes. Those who rated TV as an important source were less likely to indicate they would inform the police if they saw the various crimes mentioned in the index being committed. (For boys, r=-.16, p<.05; for girls, r=-.19, p<.01). Perceived parental attitudes were also related to this index. The more negative the attitude of parents, the less likely the child would report a crime(significance level at .05). There is a low but significant negative relationship between greater reliance on TV as a source of information about police and reluctance to inform authorities about a witnessed crime. Knowledge of legal system: As expected, there was a significant positive correlation between knowledge of arrest rights and the CSV index (p<.01). The more viewing, the more the youngsters knew their rights. Among boys, personal contact with police was also positively and significantly related to greater knowledge. No other dependent variables were related to this index. None of the six predictor variables was related to knowledge of legal terms not usually mentioned on TV.

AUTHOR'S RECOMMENDATIONS:
The author implied that more research is needed in TV as an information source and other non-TV influences on childrens' attitudes toward law enforcement.

EVALUATION:
This study begins an interesting exploratory look at the relationship between children's viewing of crime shows and their knowledge/beliefs about law enforcement. The sample size, 371, provides a large number of respondents, but there was no mention of how the sample was selected. The use of questionnaires by fifth grade school children raises questions of how these were administered. This is not given in the article. Other definitions and selection procedures such as "crime shows" and white/blue collar communities raise more methodological questions. The analysis, relying totally on correlation matrices, is associative only and cannot help to establish causal links or sort out inter-item influences. This study was published in the spring of 1974 which makes the findings quite dated. The issues raised are interesting ones which need more sophisticated analysis, a broader respondent base, and more recent replication. (CSPV Abstract - Copyright © 1992-2007 by the Center for the Study and Prevention of Violence, Institute of Behavioral Science, Regents of the University of Colorado)

KW - 1970s
KW - New York
KW - Grade 5
KW - Elementary School Student
KW - Late Childhood
KW - Exposure to Violence
KW - Media Violence Effects
KW - Television Viewing
KW - Television Violence
KW - Child Attitudes
KW - Child Perceptions
KW - Child Ideology
KW - Attitudes Toward Police
KW - Law Enforcement

NEW SEARCH


All SafetyLit records are available for automatic download to Zotero & Mendeley
Print